Az Eszterházy Károly Tanárképző Főiskola Tudományos Közleményei. 1994. [Vol. 2.] Eger Journal of American Studies. (Acta Academiae Paedagogicae Agriensis : Nova series ; Tom. 22)

STUDIES - Csaba Czeglédi: On the Distribution of Infinitival and Gerundive Complements in English

Interestingly enough, it seems that the reverse is the case with re­spect to pseudo-clefting: gerunds cannot, but some infinitives can appear in the focus of a pseudo-cleft: (37) *What Mary likes is writing papers. (38) What Mary wants is to write papers. I do not have an explanation for this fact but I suspect that the answer lies in some still not clearly understood differences between the semantic effects of clefting and pseudo-clefting. Below I present a few examples highlighting (by capitalization) the elements that trigger the respective implicit contrasts as described above, suggesting that the meaning expressed by the expressions printed un­changed in the examples is kept constant in the contrasts implied. They are arranged in three groups: Group A contains sentences with matrix verbs that take either infinitival or gerundive complements; Group B is a list of sentences whose matrix verbs allow only infinitives; and Group C contains examples with matrix verbs that take only gerundive nonfinite complements. Group A Examples with matrix verbs for which the choice between infinitival and gerundive complementation is available. (2a-b) repeated here as (39) a. Did you THINK to ask Brown? b. Did you think OF ASKING BROWN? (3a-b) repeated here as (40) a. I DECIDED to go. b. I decided ON GOING. (8a-b) repeated here as (41) a. Sheila TRIED to bribe the jailor. b. Sheila tried BRIBING THE JAILOR (12a-b) repeated here as (42) a. He TRIED to fry the mushrooms. b. He tried FRYING the mushrooms. 26

Next

/
Oldalképek
Tartalom